There is a long tradition of linking climate to psychological and behavioral variation, from early theories of environmental determinism to more recent cross-cultural studies showing associations between temperature, seasonality, and personality traits. Building on these ideas, an Arctic-origins framework has been proposed by David Sun (2025), suggesting that survival in harsh, highly seasonal ecologies may have shaped patterns of social and emotional functioning over evolutionary time. Although this framework does not make polygenic predictions, it provides a foundation for testable hypotheses. To bring it into dialogue with genetics, I translated the framework into a Big-Five–based operationalization: if these ecologies left a long shadow, we should observe population-level genomic signals consistent (on average) with lower Extraversion and Neuroticism and higher Agreeableness. This translation is my interpretation, intended to generate measurable predictions from ancient and modern DNA. The paper is currently under review, and additional methodological details and references will be available in the forthcoming preprint
Theoretical Framework: Mechanisms and Predictions
Evolutionary Pathway from Arctic Ecology to Personality
The proposed mechanism linking harsh Arctic conditions to personality shifts operates through multiple selective pressures acting over thousands of generations. Here, ‘Arctic’ is used in a broad sense to include not only the Arctic proper but also sub-Arctic regions and other environments characterized by prolonged cold winters. In the analyses, latitude was included as a continuous variable, since it serves as a strong proxy for cold-climate exposure and is mechanistically tied to both UV intensity and photoperiod variation.
Resource Scarcity and Cooperation: Theoretical models from behavioral ecology demonstrate that environmental adversity and uncertainty favor the emergence of cooperation, with mathematical arguments and simulations showing that individuals in more adverse environments perceive resources as more unpredictable, and this unpredictability favors cooperative strategies (Andras, Lazarus, & Roberts, 2007; Ibbotson et al., 2022). While direct archaeological evidence for personality-relevant behaviors is limited, ethnographic studies of circumpolar populations provide compelling support: Inuit communities across Alaska and Canada maintain elaborate food-sharing networks where communities experiencing harvest difficulties receive food from others (Kishigami, 2006; Collings et al., 2016). Research on Canadian Inuit food systems shows that sharing channels country food to individuals who do not hunt and provides households with buffering against climate risks (Ready et al., 2018). Hunting, harvesting, and sharing of country food play an integral role in social cohesion and cultural continuity for Inuit communities, with country food remaining central to Inuit cultural identity (Kishigami, 2006; Ethnology, 2002). These documented patterns of resource sharing, community-wide risk buffering, and conflict-avoidance strategies align closely with behavioral predictions of higher Agreeableness and lower Neuroticism under conditions of resource uncertainty.
Emotional Regulation Under Stress: High Neuroticism - characterized by anxiety, emotional volatility, and stress reactivity - is particularly maladaptive in environments where survival margins are thin. Elevated stress responses in subsistence contexts can undermine decision-making, leading to impulsive resource consumption, loss of focus during extended hunting or food processing, or panic responses in high-stakes situations (Lahey, 2009; Ormel et al., 2013). In contrast, the ecological demands of Arctic life - including tracking seasonal resources, planning for months-long cold periods, and coordinating food-sharing and cooperative labor in confined social settings - would favor emotional stability and low trait anxiety (Kishigami, 2006; Ready et al., 2018). Under such conditions, individuals with lower Neuroticism would have been better equipped to maintain the foresight, planning, and cooperative social functioning essential for survival.
Energy Conservation and Social Dynamics. Lower Extraversion may emerge from the metabolic economics of extreme environments. Extraversion is associated with sensation-seeking, social assertiveness, and elevated activity levels (Costa & McCrae, 1992), traits that can be energetically costly when calories are scarce and subsistence activity must be carefully rationed. In Arctic subsistence contexts, where food acquisition is highly seasonal and energy expenditures are tightly constrained, excessive activity or novelty-seeking would reduce survival margins (Kishigami, 2006). Moreover, the small, tightly knit groups necessitated by Arctic living make aggressive self-promotion disruptive rather than advantageous. Ethnographic accounts indicate that leadership often shifted toward consensus-building and conflict avoidance rather than dominance competition, with cooperation and food sharing serving as central mechanisms for social stability (Collings et al., 2016; Ready et al., 2018).
Seasonal Affective Pressures. The extreme photoperiod variations of high latitudes also create unique selective pressures on behavior and personality. Populations needed to maintain group cohesion and effective functioning through months of near-total darkness, conditions that are strongly associated with mood dysregulation and heightened psychological strain (Rosenthal et al., 1984). Under these circumstances, traits associated with emotional stability and reduced stimulation-seeking - namely, lower baseline anxiety (low Neuroticism) and lower Extraversion - would be adaptive, while maintaining prosocial orientation (high Agreeableness) would help prevent social breakdown during prolonged periods of isolation (Lahey, 2009; Ormel et al., 2013).
Figure 1. Schematic representation of David Sun’s model.
The methods
I built polygenic scores (PGS) for Agreeableness, Extraversion, and Neuroticism from large genome-wide studies. A PGS is one summary number that adds up thousands of tiny DNA effects linked to a trait; each effect is minuscule on its own, but together they sketch extremely faint group-level tendencies. To keep the signal clean, I used conservative quality control, including clumping/thresholding—in practice, keeping one representative variant per genomic neighborhood and dropping weaker, redundant ones.
I utilized the largest and most recent GWAS on the Big Five personality traits from Schwaba et al. (2025), encompassing 46 cohorts with 611,000 to 1.14 million participants of primarily European and African ancestry. These polygenic scores accounted for approximately 9% of the variance in each trait, increasing to 10%–16% after correcting for measurement unreliability. Importantly, genetic influences on personality showed high consistency across geographic regions, raters (self vs. close others), age groups, and assessment methods, though Agreeableness demonstrated lower consistency across geography and rating instruments. A critical validation was that polygenic scores maintained predictive validity in within-family analyses, suggesting minimal confounding by shared environmental factors and supporting genuine genetic effects rather than population stratification artifacts.
I applied these scores to modern genomes from many populations and to ancient genomes spanning the Late Pleistocene into the Holocene. In Western Eurasia, I modeled how scores change through time with a hinge regression and placed two knots where climate history gives us compelling breakpoints: a strict one at the end of the LGM (19,000 years ago) and a second at the beginning of the Holocene (12,000 years ago). I’ll include the plots in the post; the first knot anchors the deglacial onset, and the second captures the transition to truly interglacial conditions. The 19 kya choice follows the canonical timing of LGM termination in global ice extent, while the 12 kya knot aligns with the Holocene start at ~11.7 kya after the Younger Dryas cold reversal (Fig. 2).
Why that second knot at 12 kya?
• The LGM was globally ~6.1 °C colder than preindustrial; ice-sheet retreat from LGM maxima spans 26.5–19/20 kya, so 19 kya is a defensible “strict” deglacial hinge (Tierney et al., 2020).
• From 20→10 kya, the mean global ocean warmed ~2.57 ± 0.24 °C, i.e., only part of the LGM→Holocene contrast had been made up by 12 kya (Bereiter et al., 2018).
• The deglaciation was stepwise: a Northern-Hemisphere Bølling–Allerød warm pulse 14.7–12.9 kya, followed by the Younger Dryas cold reversal 12.9–11.7 kya; the abrupt shift to sustained interglacial warmth occurs at ~11.7 kya—the Holocene start (He et al., 2021).
Together, these data support treating 12 kya as a second hinge: by then conditions were less cold than LGM, but not yet stably warm until the Holocene begins (Osman et al, 2021).
Figure 2.

In Eastern Eurasia, pre-Holocene coverage is thinner, so instead of forcing a time trend I tested how specific ancestry components (e.g., Arctic/Siberian, Jōmon, Tibetan, Northeast Chinese) relate to the same scores within the Holocene, alongside geography. Finally, for the modern snapshot, I checked whether the three traits move together on a single axis—useful as a compact “personality factor” to compare across places and times.
The results
Western Eurasia: a Late-Pleistocene → Holocene pivot
At the population level, correlations among mean polygenic scores for the three traits were modest (Extraversion–Neuroticism: r = 0.27; Agreeableness–Neuroticism: r = −0.19; Extraversion–Agreeableness: r = −0.03). Tests of factorability were mixed: the KMO statistic was borderline (0.503), but Bartlett’s test supported proceeding (χ²(3) = 10.50, p = 0.015). Parallel analysis indicated a single latent dimension. A one-factor solution, extracted via principal-axis factoring, yielded a weak but coherent factor dominated by Neuroticism, with smaller, opposing loadings of Extraversion and Agreeableness. For interpretability, factor scores were oriented such that higher values reflect greater Agreeableness and emotional stability (i.e., low Neuroticism and Extraversion).
Approaching the LGM, genetics linked to lower Extraversion and lower Neuroticism strengthen, with higher Agreeableness moving in the complementary direction. After the Ice Age, those trends ease or reverse. The bend captured by the two-knot model fits a picture of selection tightening in the cold and relaxing in the warmer Holocene (Fig. 3).
Figure 3.

Latitude is still a strong, independent predictor of the “Arctic” Personality Factor even after controlling for time (Years BP), the hinge break (at 12k or 19k BP), and coverage. In both hinge specifications, the latitude coefficient is ~+0.06 per degree (p < 1.5×10⁻⁷). Time trends are positive toward the LGM (Date_kyr > 0), and the hinge term is negative, indicating the slope weakens/turns after the knot. Coverage is also positively associated. Overall fit is solid (R² ≈ 0.42 for the 12k model; 0.39 for the 19k model), with highly significant F-tests.
Table 1.

Models with individual personality factors
At the level of the individual personality factors (A,E,N), Latitude had a significant negative effect on Extraversion (β= -0.13) and on Neuroticism (β= -0.065), but a non significant one on Agreeableness (β= -0.027). Years Before Present had opposite effects on Agreeableness (β=0.05) and Neuroticism (β= -0.065), indicating more ancient populations in Europe were more agreeable and less neurotic.
Eastern Eurasia: ancestry beats latitude
In Holocene-era Eastern samples, lineage history matters more than simple latitude. Ancestry components tied to interior, highly seasonal Northeast Asian groups align with the “agreeable & emotionally steady” profile; once ancestry is in the model, latitude adds little.
Figure 4 illustrates the effects of ancestry and geographical variables on the personality factor PGS. Tibetan, Northeast Chinese, and Jomon ancestries show positive effects on the PF, whereas the reference group - Southern Chinese - exhibits a comparatively negative effect. Interestingly, Southeast Asian ancestry also displays a positive effect, an unexpected result that does not align with the Arctic model and suggests the influence of additional selection pressures beyond climate. Despite not being very north, Tibetans live at high altitude, so they are subject to similar climatic pressures.
Figure 4.
Ethnographic/anthropological note:
Jōmon (Japan’s deep-time hunter-gatherers)
The Jōmon were the prehistoric peoples of the Japanese archipelago who lived mainly as sedentary hunter-gatherer-fishers from roughly 14,000–300 BCE. They’re famous for some of the world’s earliest pottery (with the rope-impressed “jō-mon” pattern), pit-dwellings, shell middens, and rich foraging economies along coasts and forests. Genetically, ancient Jōmon form a distinct Northeast Asian lineage that contributes ancestry to modern Japanese—especially Ainu and Ryukyuan populations—with later Yayoi farmers from the continent adding additional layers.
Interestingly, Jomon ancestry (the component extracted via ADMIXTURE, not the samples that were labelled “Jomon”)had a strong positive effect on Agreeableness, as shown in the effect size plot below:
Figure 5.
In everyday Japan, the behaviors we label “manners” map neatly onto Agreeableness—especially its cooperation, altruism, and modesty facets. Norms like wa (harmony) and omoiyari (anticipating others’ needs) encourage people to smooth conflict, read context, and act considerately. Enryo (restraint) and ritualized politeness - keigo in speech, bowing, punctuality, orderly queuing, routine apologies, and thoughtful omiyage (gifts) - all reward low antagonism and high cooperativeness. Decision-making practices such as nemawashi (quiet, pre-meeting consensus-building) favor accommodation over confrontation, while the ideal of meiwaku o kakenai (don’t impose on others) keeps everyday interactions prosocial and other-oriented. Even indirect communication - prioritizing the relationship over bluntness - reflects a preference to preserve harmony. Taken together, a Jōmon-linked tilt toward Agreeableness - layered with these long-standing cultural scripts - offers a coherent backdrop for the higher Agreeableness signal observed in modern Japanese.
Modern snapshot: a coordinated factor
In today’s populations, Agreeableness, Extraversion, and Neuroticism cohere into one factor (higher = more agreeable & emotionally steady, lower extraversion/neuroticism). East Asians and many Indigenous American groups sit relatively high on that factor.
PF rises from low latitudes into the mid-latitudes (~30–40°), then flattens and dips slightly at higher northern latitudes (Fig.6). Low-latitude African groups cluster at the bottom of the scale; many East Asian (and some American) groups in the mid-latitudes sit near the peak; European populations from ~45–60° scatter around the mean with a slight downturn. The wide band at higher latitudes indicates greater between-population variability there, consistent with a curvilinear, not strictly linear, latitude effect.
Figure 6.
Continental ancestry almost entirely explains cross-population differences in the Personality Factor Score. Regressing PFS on superpopulation (East Asians as the reference) accounts for 97.5% of the variance (F(5,26)=199.97, p<.001), and all groups differ significantly from East Asians: African shows the largest negative shift (β=−2.80), then Middle Eastern/North African (β=−1.35), with more moderate negatives for European (β=−0.72) and South Asian (β=−0.67). In other words, East Asians sit highest on the factor, and the superpopulation labels capture nearly all between-group signal.
What’s next
I’m expanding the ancient panel (especially older Siberian samples), swapping latitude for direct climate variables, stress-testing with alternative GWAS and within-family estimates, and building simple demography + selection models to gauge plausible magnitudes.
References
Andras, P., Lazarus, J., & Roberts, G. (2007). Environmental adversity and uncertainty favour cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274(1620), 959–962. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.0071
Bereiter, B., Shackleton, S., Baggenstos, D., Kawamura, K., & Severinghaus, J. (2018). Mean global ocean temperatures during the last glacial transition. Nature, 553(7686), 39–44. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25152
Collings, P., Marten, M. G., Pearce, T., & Young, A. G. (2016). Country food sharing networks, household structure, and implications for understanding food insecurity in Arctic Canada. Ecology of Food and Nutrition, 55(1), 30–49. https://doi.org/10.1080/03670244.2015.1072812
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Enloe, J. (2003). Food sharing past and present: Archaeological evidence for economic and social interactions. Before Farming, 2003(1). https://doi.org/10.3828/bfarm.2003.1.1
He, C., Liu, Z., Otto-Bliesner, B. L., Brady, E. C., Zhu, C., Tomas, R., Buizert, C., & Severinghaus, J. P. (2021). Abrupt Heinrich Stadial 1 cooling missing in Greenland oxygen isotopes. Science advances, 7(25), eabh1007. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.abh1007
Ibbotson, L., Andras, P., & Roberts, G. (2022). Dying to cooperate: The role of environmental harshness in human collaboration. Adaptive Behavior, 30(2), 125–139. https://doi.org/10.1177/10597123221080567
Inuit Identity in the Canadian Arctic. (2002). Ethnology, 41(3), 239–256. https://www.jstor.org/stable/3773946
Kishigami, N. (2006). Contemporary Inuit food sharing and hunter support program of Nunavik, Canada. Senri Ethnological Studies, 75, 171–192. https://doi.org/10.15021/00001344
Lahey, B. B. (2009). Public health significance of neuroticism. American Psychologist, 64(4), 241–256. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015309
Ormel, J., Jeronimus, B. F., Kotov, R., Riese, H., Bos, E. H., Hankin, B., … Oldehinkel, A. J. (2013). Neuroticism and common mental disorders: Meaning and utility of a complex relationship. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(5), 686–697. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.00
Osman, M.B., Tierney, J.E., Zhu, J. et al. Globally resolved surface temperatures since the Last Glacial Maximum. Nature 599, 239–244 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-03984-4
Ready, E., Collings, P., & Luo, J. (2018). Sharing-based social capital associated with harvest production and wealth in the Canadian Arctic. PLOS ONE, 13(3), e0193759. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0193759
Rosenthal, N. E., Sack, D. A., Gillin, J. C., Lewy, A. J., Goodwin, F. K., Davenport, Y., … Wehr, T. A. (1984). Seasonal affective disorder: A description of the syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41(1), 72–80. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1984.01790120076010
Schwaba, T. et al. (2025). Robust inference and widespread genetic correlates from a large-scale genetic association study of human personality. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2025.05.16.648988
Sun, D. (2025). Arctic instincts? The Late Pleistocene Arctic origins of East Asian psychology.Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 19(3), 227–265. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000373
Tierney, J.E., Zhu, J., King, J. et al. Glacial cooling and climate sensitivity revisited. Nature 584, 569–573 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2617-x